Teacher training programs often encourage us to brighten our classrooms with lively, colorful, personal, and uplifting stuff:
Students’ art work.
Anchor charts.
Word walls.
You know the look.
We certainly hope that these decorations invite our students in and invigorate their learning. (We might even have heard that “enriched environments promote learning.”)
At the same time, we might worry that all those decorations could distract our students from important cognitive work.
So, which is it? Do decorations distract or inspire? Do they promote learning or inhibit learning? If only we had research on this question…
Part I: Early Research
But wait: we DO have research on this objection.
Back in 2014, a team led by Dr. Anna Fisher asked if classroom decorations might be “Too Much of a Good Thing.”
They worked with Kindergarten students, and found that — sure enough — students who learned in highly-decorated rooms paid less attention and learned less than others in “sparsely” decorated classroom.
Since then, other researchers have measured students’ performance on specific mental tasks in busy environments, or in plain environments.
The results: the same. A busy visual field reduced working memory and attention scores, compared to plain visual environments.
It seems that we have a “brain-based” answer to our question:
Classroom decorations can indeed be “too much of a good thing.”
Taken too far, they distract students from learning.
Part II: Important Doubts
But wait just one minute…
When I present this research in schools, I find that teachers have a very plausible question.
Sure: those decorations might distract students at first. But, surely the students get used to them.
Decorations might make learning a bit harder at first. But ultimately students WON’T be so distracted, and they WILL feel welcomed, delighted, and inspired.
In this theory, a small short-term problem might well turn into a substantial long-term benefit.
And I have to be honest: that’s a plausible hypothesis.
Given Fisher’s research (and that of other scholars), I think the burden of proof is on people who say that decorations are not distracting. But I don’t have specific research to contradict those objections.
Part III: The Researchers Return
So now maybe you’re thinking: “why don’t researchers study this specific question”?
I’ve got good news: they just did.
In a recently-published study, another research team (including Fisher, and led by Dr. Karrie Godwin, who helped in the 2014 study) wondered if students would get used to the highly decorated classrooms.
Research isn’t research if we don’t use fancy terminology, so they studied “habituation.” As in: did students habituate to the highly decorated classrooms?
In the first half of their study, researchers again worked with Kindergarteners. Students spent five classes studying science topics in plainly decorated classrooms. (The visual material focused only on the topic being presented.)
Then they spent ten classes studying science topics in highly decorated classrooms. (These decorations resembled typical classroom decorations: posters, charts, artwork, etc.)
Unsurprisingly (based on the 2014 study), students were more distractable in the decorated classroom.
But: did they get used to the decorations? Did they become less distractable over time? Did they habituate?
The answer: a little bit.
In other words: students were less distractible than they initially were in the decorated classroom. But they were still more distractible than in the sparsely decorated room.
Even after ten classes, students hadn’t fully habituated.
Part IV: Going Big
This 2-week study with kindergarteners, I think, gives us valuable information.
We might have hoped that students will get used to decorations, and so benefit from their welcoming uplift (but not be harmed by their cognitive cost). So far, this study deflates that hope.
However, we might still hold out a possibility:
If students partially habituate over two weeks, won’t they fully habituate eventually? Won’t the habituation trend continue?
Team Godwin wanted to answer that question too. They ran yet another study in primary school classrooms.
This study had somewhat different parameters (the research nitty-gritty gets quite detailed). But the headline is: this study lasted 15 weeks.
Depending on the school system you’re in, that’s between one-third and one-half of a school year.
How much did the students habituate to the visual distractions?
The answer: not at all.
The distraction rate was the same after fifteen weeks as it was at the beginning of the year.
To my mind, that’s an AMAZING research finding.
Putting It Together
At this point, I think we have a compelling research story.
Despite our training — and, perhaps, despite our love of decoration — we have a substantial body of research suggesting that over-decorated classrooms interfere with learning.
The precise definition of “over-decorated” might take some time to sort out. And, the practical problems of putting up/taking down relevant learning supports deserves thought and sympathetic exploration.
However: we shouldn’t simply hope away the concern that young students can be distracted by the environment.
And we shouldn’t trust that they’ll get used to the busy environment.
Instead, we should deliberately create environments that welcome students, inspire students, and help students concentrate and learn.
Fisher, A. V., Godwin, K. E., & Seltman, H. (2014). Visual environment, attention allocation, and learning in young children: When too much of a good thing may be bad. Psychological science, 25(7), 1362-1370.
Godwin, K. E., Leroux, A. J., Seltman, H., Scupelli, P., & Fisher, A. V. (2022). Effect of Repeated Exposure to the Visual Environment on Young Children’s Attention. Cognitive Science, 46(2), e13093.
Thank you for this, Andrew. I love how your posts often explore potential ways of “holding out”. Should the research start to move in the opposite direction, my “hold out” is a workload argument. Novice teachers have a lot to focus on in their first year of teaching. They might have always dreamed of decorating their first classroom, but the alarming rates that new teachers drop out of teaching tells me they’d be better served if an expert directed their attention towards the activities that will yield fast results – often related to behavior and classroom routines, and delivering that standard lesson. If new teachers (I’m talking elementary, mostly) have the energy to come in August 1 to decorate a Pinterest classroom, we might want to consider tapping into that energy by forming new teacher boot camps and PLCs, rather than seeing it wasted on pointless work that may actually harm learning.
Under Part IV: Going Big , in the first sentence you refer to “This 2-week study using kindergarteners…” Are you referring to the study you just discussed in the previous section? It didn’t have a 2 week phase, but a 5 and 10 week phase. I’m confused about which study you are referring to.
Thank you for pointing out this confusion.
The first part of the study had a 5-class and a 10-class phase: those two phases added up to two weeks. The initial post could/should have been clearer on this point.
While your research is compelling, let us not forget that that the developing brain of a five-year-old vs. that of a middle school student or adolescent student are quite different. Have these other populations been studied? I would be curious to know what the results are if they have been studied. Another factor to consider is if a youngster has undiagnosed ADHD, therefore unmedicated, this too will affect the results.
Excellent points.
Check out the link under “The results: the same.” This research team showed (in a different way) that adolescents are also distracted by visual environment.
And, indeed, ADHD will influence distractibility.
As you suggest, no one study can answer all questions. We must look at several studies together before we reach confident conclusions. In fact, my interest in this Godwin study arose because it deepened and extended an already-important pool of research.
As a veteran kindergarten teacher, I am curious about how “ learning” was defined in the study. I decorate my classroom expecting the children to be engaged by the photos and art. I believe that it’s developmentally appropriate for kindergartners to be curious about their visual environment and I provide engaging and thought provoking images. I seem to be able to get their attention when I want it, but then, I do not rely very much on whole group, teacher-led instruction.
In the 2014 study, researchers tested students by asking questions and having students point at the picture that showed the correct answer. Here’s their description:
“The children were asked to select the correct answer from four pictorial response options (one correct answer and three lures). All response options were novel (i.e., four pictures the children had not seen during the lesson) to ensure that the children were not merely selecting a familiar answer.”
The more recent studies focused on attention and habituation, but didn’t measure learning. (Of course, both common sense and the 2014 study suggest that we learn less when we don’t pay as much attention.)
Thank you so much for researching the question burning in all of us for years. I will use this article as a springboard to setting up classrooms next school year as we move to some normalcy after COVID.